So continuing on from the last blog, might
our warning systems on tipping points have been hindered?
Early warning systems have been proposed to
forecast the possibility of a critical transition, such as the eutrophication
of a lake, the collapse of a coral reef or the end of a glacial period. Because
such transitions often unfold on temporal and spatial scales that can be
difficult to approach by experimental manipulations, research has often relied
on historical observations as a source of natural experiments.
Here, Boettiger and Hastings, examine a
critical difference between selecting systems for study based on the fact that
we have observed a critical transition and those systems for which we wish to
forecast the approach of a transition. This difference arises by conditionally
selecting systems known to experience a transition of some sort and failing to
account for the bias this introduces - a statistical error known as the
prosecutor’s fallacy. The term is however most often associated with
prosecuting lawyers arguing for the guilt of a defendant in a criminal trial.
By analyzing simulated systems that have
experienced transitions purely by chance, they reveal an elevated rate of
false-positives in common warning signal statistics.
The attempts to detect early warning signs
for critical transitions are based on the concept of deteriorating environment
as embodied in a changing parameter (Scheffer et. al., 2009), which is a
different kind of transition than the alternative of stochastic system (i.e.
non- deterministic, so probabilities are used to work out potential outcomes)
in an environment that is otherwise constant and exhibiting no directional
change. When trying to use historical data to understand critical transitions,
we often do not know which category, changing environment or simply chance, an
observed large change falls into, which leads to uncertainty.
Boettiger and Hastings have shown here that
systems that undergo rare sudden transitions owing to chance look statistically
different from their counterparts that do not, even though they are driven by
the same stochastic process (non-deterministic).
In particular, such conditionally selected
examples are more likely to show signs associated with an early warning of an
approaching tipping point, such as increasing variance or increasing
autocorrelation, as measured by Kendall’s (used to measure the
association between two measured quantities).
This increases the risk of false positives
- cases in which a warning signal being tested appears to have successfully
detected an underlying change in the system leading to a tipping point, when in
fact the example comes instead from a stable system with no underlying change
in parameters.
It does seem tempting to argue that this
bias towards positive detection in historical examples is not problematic each
of these systems did indeed collapse; so the increased probability of exhibiting
warning signals could be taken as successful detection. Unfortunately this
isn’t the case. At the moment the forecast is made, these systems are not
likely to transition, because they experience a strong pull towards the
original stable state. As the system gets farther from its stable point, it is
more likely to draw a random step that returns it towards the stable point.
However of course there is also the chance that it will continue away from its
original stable point, thus any systems that do cross a tipping point would do
so rather quickly.
The authors do also go on to demonstrate a
model-based approach that is less subject to this bias than those more commonly
used in summary statistics as well as highlight the fact that experimental
studies with replicates avoid this pitfall entirely – largely through running
many models and improving knowledge of the system to remove bias. However I think that’s enough for now and
this new method is still to be fully evaluated and/or used by the scientific
community.
Read the full paper at: http://rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org/content/279/1748/4734.full.pdf+html
Reference:
Boettiger, C. and Hastings, A. (2012) Proc.
R. Soc. B vol. 279, no. 1748. 4734–4739.
Scheffer, M. et al. 2009 Early-warning
signals for critical transitions. Nature 461, 53–59. (doi:10.1038/nature08227)
No comments:
Post a Comment